COMPETITIVENESS AMONG JAPANESE, 2005-Houston.pdf

(449 KB) Pobierz
PsychologicalReports, 2005,97,205-212.
O
Psychological Reports 2005
COMPETITIVENESS AMONG JAPANESE, CHINESE, AND
AMERICAN UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS
'
JOHN M. HOUSTON, PAUL B. HARRIS,
ROBERT MOORE,
AND
REBECCA BRUMMETT
Rollins College
HIDEKI KAMETANI
Saitama Institute of Technology
Summay.-Although
research indicates that competitiveness, defined as the de-
sire to win in interpersonal situations, is an important individual difference that influ-
ences a range of social interactions, little research has focused on competitiveness in
cultures outside the United States. This study investigated competitiveness in three cul-
tures by comparing Chinese (n
=
61), Japanese
( n
=
232), and American (n
=
161) un-
dergraduate college students. Nationality and sex were compared on two scales of the
revised Competitiveness Index. Analysis indicated that American students scored high-
er on Enjoyment of Competitiveness than Chinese and Japanese students, but no dif-
ference was found on Contentiousness. Men scored higher than women on Enjoyment
of Competition but not on Contentiousness. The findings indicate that sex and cul-
tural patterns influence some but not all aspects of competitiveness.
Competitiveness is an important individual difference variable that influ-
ences social interaction across a broad range of situations. Although research-
ers have examined competitiveness in a variety of samples from the United
States, including trial lawyers and nurses (Houston, Farese,
&
LaDu, 1992),
airline reservation representatives (Helrnreich, Sawin,
&
Carsrud, 1986), pro-
fessional tennis players (Houston, Carter,
&
Smither
,
1997), collegiate ath-
letes (Gill
&
Dzewaltowski, 19881, and undergraduate college students (Mar-
tin
&
Larsen, 1976; Griffin-Pierson, 1990; Smither
&
Houston, 1992), sur-
prisingly little research has focused on samples from cultures outside of the
United States (for an exception, see Lynn, 1991).
Competitiveness
Research on competitiveness dates back to the origins of experimental
social psychology and the pioneering work of Triplett (1897) who investi-
gated such concepts as competitive instincts, mental attitudes during perfor-
mance, and "the desire to beat7' (p. 528). Over fifty years later, Deutsch
'The authors thank Ruri Yokoyama and Nicholas Williams for their he1 in the back-transla-
tion process and Rumi Mizuma, Kim Kochurka, and Elgin Mellown for tKeir help in the com-
let ion
of this project. Address enquiries to John M. Houston, Department of Ps chology,
Rollins College, 1000 Holt Ave., Winter Park,
FL
12789 or e-mail (jhouston@rollins.e&.
206
J.
M.
HOUSTON,
ET
AL.
(1949) initiated a new phase in the study of competitiveness by examining
winning from the perspective of goal relationships as opposed to the instinc-
tual concepts proposed by Triplett (1897). According to Deutsch, context
and reward structure play key roles in the expression of competitive behav-
ior by defining the way valued rewards are distributed within group settings.
In his model, competition is highest under "winner take all" conditions in
which only one group member receives all the available rewards. Although
the reward distribution approach has produced a number of insights into
behavior in competitive and cooperative situations, it overlooks competitive-
ness as an individual difference variable.
More recent approaches (e.g., Gill
&
Deeter, 1988; Griffin-Pierson,
1990; Houston, Harris, McIntire,
&
Francis, 2002) conceptualize competi-
tiveness as a personality characteristic that influences behavior across a wide
variety of social domains including work, sports, and interpersonal relation-
ships. Based on the conceptual framework provided by achievement motiva-
tion research, competitiveness is generally defined as "the desire to win in
interpersonal situations'' (Helmreich
&
Spence, 1978, p. 4). Unlike Need for
Achievement which relies on an internalized standard of excellence (Smither
&
Houston, 1992)) competitiveness requires the perceived presence of a ri-
val or group of competitors who serve as performance standards for the in-
dividual.
Few researchers have examined the relationship between competitive-
ness and culture. One exception is a study of work attitudes and rates of eco-
nomic growth in 43 countries by Lynn (1991) which found that competitive-
ness is positively related to economic growth. In a reanalysis of these data,
Furnham, Kirkcaldy, and Lynn (1994) found that Asian and Eastern coun-
tries, e.g., China, India, Israel, and Japan, scored higher on competitiveness
than countries from the Americas, e.g., Argentina, Brazil, Colombia, and the
United States. While these findings underscore the significant role competi-
tiveness plays in economic development around the world, the grouping of
countries by geographical region may mask significant cultural differences.
Individualism
and
Collectivism
Although China, Japan and the United States differ along a number of
sociocultural dimensions, the individualism-collectivism dialectic provides a
particularly useful conceptual framework for investigating competitiveness in
these three cultures. Triandis, McCusker, and Hui (1990) maintained that
individualistic cultures tend to place priority on achieving one's own goals
over group goals and define identity in terms of personal attributes. Collec-
tivistic cultures generally give priority to the goals of the group and define
identity in terms of the group. Research by Hofstede (1980, 1991, 2001) in-
dicates that individualism is particularly high in the United States while col-
COMPETITIVENESS IN THREE CULTURES
207
lectivism is very high in China and moderately high in Japan. In a review of
the literature on individualism and collectivism Triandis (1995) found that
American individualists link self-reliance with competition, Japanese collec-
tivists tend to emphasize in-group harmony, and East Asian collectivists, e.g.,
Chinese, try to avoid confrontation that would cause anyone to lose face.
Sex
Dgferences in Competitiveness
Research suggests that men score higher on competitiveness than wom-
en in the U.S. and in many other nations (Helmreich,
et
al.,
1986; Gill, 1988;
Houston,
et
al., 1997). Lynn (1991, 1993) reported that men scored signifi-
cantly higher than women in 20 countries, including China, Japan, and Tai-
wan. Conversely, women scored higher in only one country (Iraq).
Although the factors contributing to sex differences in competitiveness
remain a matter of debate, two metatheories of sex differences, evolutionary
psychology and social structure theory, suggest that sex differences in com-
petitiveness should generalize across cultures. Evolutionary psychologists
(e.g., Buss, 1995) argued that sex differences in many psychological disposi-
tions evolved through male competition for sexual access to females and
through female choice of mates. Since intrasexual competition was critical in
obtaining access to women, men evolved dispositions favoring competition,
aggression, and risk-taking. In contrast, women evolved dispositions favoring
nurturing and a preference for long-term mates who could provide resources
to support a family. Because these sex differences are proposed to be the re-
sult of adaptive mechanisms that evolved in primeval environments, they are
believed to be universal or near-universal in contemporary cultures.
Social structuralists (e.g., Eagly
&
Wood, 1999) propose a very different
explanation, attributing sex differences in competitiveness to differences in
power, status, and control of resources between men and women in many
societies around the world. Thus, while men's accommodation to roles with
higher power and status leads to more dominant behavior, women's accom-
modation to roles with lower power and status leads to more subordinate
behavior (Ridgeway
&
Diekema, 1992). Since greater power and status tends
to be associated with male-dominated roles across cultures, corresponding
sex differences in competitiveness should also be found across cultures.
Drawing on the theoretical framework of collectivism and individualism
outlined by Triandis,
et
al. (1990), it was hypothesized that American under-
graduates would have higher competitiveness scores than Chinese and Japa-
nese undergraduates.
Based on predictions from both evolutionary psychology and social
structure theory, it was hypothesized that men should score higher on com-
petitiveness than women in China, Japan, and the United States.
208
J.
M.
HOUSTON,
ET
AL.
Participants
A total of 454 undergraduates (61 Chinese, 232 Japanese, and 161
American) ranging in age from 18 to 28 years participated. To ensure mean-
ingful comparisons could be made between similar groups of undergradu-
ates, this study focused on four undergraduate institutions with culturally
and ethnically homogeneous student populations and enrollments under
4000. Participants in the Chinese sample (30 women and 31 men) had a
mean age of 20.9 yr. (SD= 1.4) and attended a selective residential university
located within metropolitan Beijing. The Japanese participants (102 women
and 130 men) attended one of two selective residential colleges, one in Fuku-
oka and the other in Saitama, and had a mean age of 20.4 yr. (SD
=
1.0). The
American participants (95 women and 66 men) had a mean age of 20.0 yr.
(SD
=
1.7) and attended a selective residential college located within metro-
politan Orlando, FL. American participants were primarily Euro-American
students of middle to high socioeconomic status.
Measurement
All participants completed the revised Competitiveness Index (Hous-
ton, Harris, McIntire,
&
Francis, 2002), a 14-item self-report measure design-
ed to assess the desire to win in interpersonal situations. The Index uses a
5 -
point Likert-type response format anchored by 1: strongly disagree and
5:
strongly agree. Examples of scale items include
"I
enjoy competing against
an opponent," and
"I
often remain quiet rather than risk hurting another
person." Houston,
et al.
(2002) reported that the scale has high internal
consistency
( a
=
3 7 )
and contains two reliable subscales, labeled Enjoyment
of Competition (9 items,
a=
.90) and Contentiousness (5 items,
a=
.74).
While the Chinese and American participants completed the Index in En-
glish, the Japanese participants complete a translated version of the scale in
Japanese.
The accuracy of the Japanese translation was tested using a back-trans-
lation procedure in which two bilingual researchers translated the measure
into Japanese. After the two researchers resolved minor discrepancies to form
a single translation, an undergraduate with professional experience as a trans-
lator then translated the Japanese version back into English. The back trans-
lation was then compared to the original English version by an English Pro-
fessor and judged highly consistent across all 14 items.
To assess further the equivalence of the measure across the three sam-
ples, a series of principal component analyses using a varimax rotation and
reliability analyses were conducted on the entire data set from each sample.
For the Chinese sample,
7
out of 9 items in the Enjoyment of Competition
scale loaded on the same factor with the 9 scale items having a Cronbach al-
COMPETITIVENESS IN THREE CULTURES
209
pha of
a =
.87. The 5 items of the Contentiousness scale also formed an inter-
nally consistent scale ( a
=
.77) with
4
of the 5 items loading on the same fac-
tor. In the Japanese sample, 6 of the 9 items in the Enjoyment of Competi-
tion loaded on the same factor and yielded an internally consistent scale
(a=
based on the 9 scale items. While
3
of the
5
items in the Conten-
.82)
tiousness scale loaded on the same factor, the scale items had a somewhat
low internal consistency
(a
=
.60). Although item loadings showed some vari-
ability across samples, the original scale construction was administered to
each sample.
RESULTS
Initial analyses indicated that there were significant age differences
among groups in the sample. Specifically, there was a significant main effect
for nationality
(F,,,,,2
10.33,
p
<
.001, eta2
=
.045) and a significant interaction
=
(F,,,,,
=
16.14,
p
<
.001, eta2
=
.068).
Post
hoc
tests, using Tukey's
HSD,
indi-
cating that the Chinese sample was older than the Japanese sample, and the
Japanese sample was older than the American sample (for means and stan-
dard deviations see Table
1).The
interaction showed that men were signifi-
cantly older than women in the American sample, while women were signifi-
cantly older than men in the Japanese sample. Given these differences, age
was included as a covariate in subsequent analyses.
TABLE 1
MEANS STANDARD
AND
DEVIATIONS AGE
AND
COMPETITIVENESS
FOR
SCORES
Measure
China
Japan
United States
Total
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
Age,
yr.
Women
21.2
1.8
Men
20.8
0.8
Total
21.0
1.4
Enjoyment of Competition
Women
29.3
7.1
Men
30.8
8.1
Total
30.1
7.6
Contentiousness
14.4
4.4
Women
Men
15.6
4.1
Total
15.0
4.2
Competitiveness data were analyzed using two 2
x
3
analyses of covari-
ance with sex and nationality (China, Japan and United States) as between
subjects factors and age as a covariate (for means and standard deviations
see Table 1). Analysis of the Enjoyment of Competition Scale indicated a sig-
nificant main effect for nationality
(F,,,,
=24.09,
p
=
.001, eta2
=
.098).
Post
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin